top of page

Persuasive Essay

Childhood Amnesia: Why People Forget Memories from their Childhood

       W hen people think back and try to recall memories from early childhood, most are unable to do so. Only very few memories stay intact during adult life. A theory for why people forget the majority of their youth is "childhood amnesia," the lack of childhood memories. While there are many theories as to why this phenomenon exists, three influential factors are that babies and young children have a late-developing episodic memory, differences in early reminiscing, and an absence of language and structure. The average range of the amnesia is from age two to age eight, with the age of earliest memories being 3.5 years on average (Eacott 46). Researchers, such as Bauer and Simock and Hayne, could support the suggestion that perhaps the earliest memory dates around age 3.5 because of the third theory of childhood amnesia, the absence of language. To generalize, researchers assume that the older children become, the more cognitive capabilities they possess. While the range of childhood amnesia extends to age eight, most likely, more memories are retained from age eight than age two or even earlier, extending into infancy. As children age, the extent to which children have a late-developing episodic memory, differences in early reminiscing, and an absence of language and structure changes, making these three ideas the most significant in explaining childhood amnesia. 

​

       One of the first theories for why people forget their childhood is the late development of "episodic memory." This type of memory is responsible for encoding and storing memories for specific events that have taken place (Bauer 415). Episodic memory is especially important because of its delicate and time-limited nature. Bauer contributes a meaning for this by explaining, "Episodic memory allows for rapid, even one-trial, learning of new information and for retention of information for later retrieval. As such, it provides the building blocks for cognitive growth during development and throughout the life- span" (415). Therefore, without episodic memory, a child cannot have proper development of memories, and eventually, the memories are lost. Researchers have found that children have an "identifiable semantic memory," the type of memory that deals with remembering large sets of information, such as words or numbers. When language development begins, the process goes quickly, and the children can retain many new words. However, when the children are asked to recall an event from their past, such as a birthday party or a day at school, they are stumped, unable to remember the events (Newcombe et al. 56-57). This concept is proven in a related experiment performed on "Remembering Early Childhood," in which the researchers explain that when children were shown pictures of their past and present classmates, they were only able to recognize 20% of the faces. When compared with adults, the research shows that adults could remember all of their past highschool classmates (Newcombe et al. 55-56). This study suggests that certain features of memory develop later in life. 

​

       However, this late development can be overcome or made worse based on different reminiscing styles used by mothers. Reminiscing is when parents discuss and prompt the recollection of events with their children to spur episodic memory at an earlier age (Jack et al. 496). According to a study that examines maternal reminiscing styles, there are two mainstream forms of reminiscing: elaborative and repetitive (Jack et al. 496). The researchers say, "Parents who use an elaborative reminiscing style ask their children a range of questions about the event and provide a lot of richly detailed information about the event" (Jack et al. 496). A repetitive reminiscing style, on the other hand, involves revisiting only certain aspects of an event multiple times. It is arguable that if an elaborative method is used rather than a repetitive style, the child will remember more experiences. The study provides reasoning for the advantage of an elaborative reminiscing style in saying, "...it is not simply the amount that parents say about past events that is related to their children's emerging memory abilities; rather, it is the way in which parents discuss past events… that matters for children's memory" (Jack et al. 503). For example, reminiscing styles can compare with memorization as an adult. There may be some common knowledge regarding the idea that some study methods are more effective than others, even if the less efficient study methods are practiced more often. This idea is the same with child-parent reminiscing styles. In other words, the more intricate and more often mothers quiz their children about past events, the more likely children can remember those events, thereby strengthening and sparking early development of episodic memory. 

  

       A third explanation as to why people forget childhood memories is the absence of language in infants. If there is no structure for the children to put their memories into, the memories disappear (Eacott 48). It is not until children develop necessary language skills that "autobiographical memory," the memory of one's life (Fivush 561), can become solidified into "long-term memory," memories stored in the brain ("Memory"). After all, language is thinking. For example, how could one remember that a birthday party had balloons if, as an infant, they had no sense of what a balloon was? Researchers Simcock and Hayne performed a study to see if infants could recognize particular objects and people they had seen before. In other words, they were testing to see if the children did have a partial memory. They found that the infants remembered the objects and events through what Hayne and Rovee-Collier refer to as "perceptually based attributes" (Simcock and Hayne 225). In other words, the infants find different ways to remember, such as pointing to various objects or repeating the steps of a game previously taught. Despite this, the children were unable to verbally recall what they remember (Simcock and Hayne 225). Also, as discussed previously, without recall, it is difficult for the memories to stay in long-term memory. Without a vocabulary label on the object or event, how can one go back into their memory and pull out the information? In agreement with Simock and Hayne, perhaps the brains of young children alter themselves to remember things in a different method since there is no defined language. The brain finds a way to perform its primary functions in any way possible. 

​

I       n conclusion, possible causes of childhood amnesia, the act of forgetting memories from childhood, can be attributed to physical differences and the underdevelopment of the brains of young children. Three possible reasons for people often forgetting things from their childhood are the late-development of episodic memory, differences in reminiscing styles, and the early absence of a structured language. The three reasons are intertwined. Children can remember and process "implicit memories," memories performed without direct conscious, but are unable to remember specific things and events (Newcombe et al. 56). A reason for this difficulty is the absence of language to put the encoded information into. Children may recognize information, but without language, the information gets lost over time. The information can also be lost if mothers do not use appropriate reminiscing styles. No matter how quickly young children develop language to keep their memories, if children do not practice memory retrieval with their mothers, or any family member or friend, the memory will never be ingrained into long-term memory and will be lost. Despite the intricate possibilities for explaining childhood amnesia, a study assessing the neuropsychology of memory in preschoolers adds an interesting perspective on how the simple behavior of the children could attribute to memory loss (Bauer 417). The study suggests, "Instructed to 'do whatever will help you remember later,' preschoolers do virtually nothing… They generally lack the meta-awareness of how memory works and as a result, employ few—and largely ineffective—techniques for aiding memory" (Bauer 417). In other words, researchers can examine the many reasons children have difficulty remembering things. However, perhaps when it comes down to it, children lack the drive and motivation to take hold of their cognitive abilities.

Annotated Bibliography

Bauer, Patricia J., Jacqueline S. Leventon, and Nicole L. Varga. "Neuropsychological

Assessment of Memory in Preschoolers."Neuropsychology Review, vol. 22, no. 4, 2012,

pp. 414-24. ProQuest. www.search.proquest.com/docview/126642

7202/148CB4BDB575494APQ/1?accountid=338.

 

This source discusses memory in preschoolers, specifically episodic memory. I will use this source for one of the projects later on, perhaps for the persuasive speech or teaching a lesson. It would be about why people forget things from childhood. This source would be helpful and finding the answer to that question.

 

Eacott, Madeline J. “Memory for the Events of Early Childhood.” Current 

Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 8, no. 2, 1999, pp. 46–49. JSTOR.

www.jstor.org/stable/20182556?Search=yes&resultItem

Click=true&searchText=Eacott%2C&searchText=Madeline&searchText=

J.&searchText=%22Memory+for+the+Events+of+Early+Childhood.%22&searchUri=%2Faction%2FdoBasicSearch%3FQuery%3DEacott%252C%2BMadeline%2BJ.%2B%25E2%2580%259CMemory%2Bfor%2Bthe%2BEvents%2Bof%2BEarly%2BChildhood.%25E2%2580%259D%26amp%3Bfilter%3D&ab_segments=0%2Fbasic_SYC-4631%2Ftest&refreqid=search%3A2cc01dba1dc9ceb67b64d362ff99be3a&seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents.

 

As a whole, this article examines the theory of “childhood amnesia”. While this is similar to my other sources, this article proposes an interesting contrast that could add another element to my thesis. Eacott mentions that while adults do not remember events from childhood, infants have a surprisingly strong long-term memory. It proposes a new question of why young children have great memories, but over time the memories fade. 

 

Fivush, Robyn. “The Development of Autobiographical Memory.” Annual Reviews, Annual Reviews, 16 July 2010, www.annualreviews.org/doi/full/10.1146/annurev.

Psych.121208.131702.

 

While this source contained extensive information regarding autobiographical memory and all of its implications, I used the source only for its definition regarding autobiographical memory. I do suspect that this source will be of great use to me, specifically the sections discussing early auto-biograhical memory and causes of differences in memory. I believe that in my next assignment examining adolescent memory in education, I will use this source for research, specifically the section discussing verbal recall.

 

Jack, Fiona, et al. “Maternal Reminiscing Style during Early Childhood Predicts 

the Age of Adolescents' Earliest Memories.” Child Development, vol. 80, no. 2, 2009, pp.

496–505. JSTOR. www.jstor.org/stable/29738629?Search=yes&

resultItemClick=true&searchText=%22Maternal+Reminiscing+Style+during+Early+Chil

dhood+Predicts+the+Age+of+Adolescents%27+Earliest+Memories.%22&searchUri=%2

Faction%2FdoBasicSearch%3FQuery%3D%25E2%2580%259CMaternal%2BReminisci

ng%2BStyle%2Bduring%2BEarly%2BChildhood%2BPredicts%2Bthe%2BAge%2Bof%

2BAdolescents%2527%2BEarliest%2BMemories.%25E2%2580%259D%26amp%3Bfilt

er%3D&ab_segments=0%2Fbasic_SYC-4631%2Ftest&refreqid=search%3A086babd91b

e32ee0e125c5e5f4904f2e&seq=1#Metadata_info_tab_contents.

 

This source is about an experiment performed to prove a theory of whether or not maternal and parental reminiscing affects a child's recollection/memory of their early childhood life. In the experiment, they had 17 mothers discuss early birthdays throughout the child's life and when the children were older, the children were asked to recall what they remember about the birthdays. This experiment is especially significant because this is one of the theories I believe in the most 

regarding why we remember certain events from childhood. This proves my belief.

 

“Memory.” Encyclopaedia Britannica, Encyclopaedia Britannica, Inc., 18 April 2019.

 

This source discussed the basics of memory. It provides clear information regarding what memory is and the various types of memory that exist. This is very useful because it provides information in a way that if no prior knowledge is known about memory, a good foundation is then laid with this source. 

 

Newcombe, Nora S., et al. “Remembering Early Childhood: How Much, How, and Why (Or Why Not).” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 9, no. 2, 2000, pp. 55–8. JSTOR. www.jstor.org/stable /20182623?Search=yes&resultItem Click=true&searchText=%22Remembering+Early+Childhood%3A+How+Much%2C+How%2C+and+Why+%28Or+Why+Not%29.%22&searchUri=%2Faction%2FdoBasicSearch%3FQuery%3D%25E2%2580%259CRemembering%2BEarly%2BChildhood%253A%2BHow%2BMuch%252C%2BHow%252C%2Band%2BWhy%2B%2528Or%2BWhy%2BNot%2529.%25E2%2580%259D%26amp%3Bfilter%3D&ab_segments=0%2Fbasic_SYC-4631%2Ftest&refreqid=search%3A5b9a09829b049180fdfc190899edebb3&seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents.

 

This source mainly compared the development of implicit and explicit memory in preschool age children. They then applied this knowledge to develop theories about childhood amnesia and the aspects of that theory. This source will be very useful when looking for answers regarding why we forget things from childhood and why we remember certain things in childhood. Overall, this source analyzed what children remember and do not remember during their early childhood

 

Simcock, Gabrielle, and Harlene Hayne. “Breaking the Barrier? Children Fail to Translate Their Preverbal Memories into Language.” Psychological Science, vol. 13, no. 3, 2002, pp. 225–231. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/40063711.pdf?ab_segments=0%252 Fbasic_expensive%252Fcontrol&refreqid=excelsior%3Ab59604290d1d967a1e333494ef7d57ab.

 

In this source, an experiment is performed in which the ability of young children to remember

things through verbal and nonverbal ways are tested. Their conclusion was that the children used

nonverbal methods and remembering things since they did not yet have a fully developed set of

vocabulary through language. This finding that the absence of language plays a role in memory

could help suggest a theory of why most memories from childhood are forgotten.

bottom of page